Schmuck

Sparkle, wobble, chatter. Affix reanalysis and semantic enrichment: Iteration in German, Dutch, and English. (View as pdf.)

Mirjam Schmuck, University of Mainz

The verbal suffixes –el(e)n and –er(e)n and their cognates represent a common feature of Germanic languages including German, Dutch, and English (ger tröpfeln, dut druppelen, eng trickle; ger knattern, dut knetteren, eng clatter). Being formally identical to diminutives, verbs derived with the suffix –el(e)n have also been referred to as “verbal diminutives”. Semantically, the word formation products may display a diminutive or related meaning (e.g. attenuation, pejoration, cf. Jurafsky 1996, Weidhaas & Schmid 2015). However, there are many instances where diminutive semantics are absent and iterative meaning is predominant instead (ger stammeln, dut stamelen, eng stumble). How the derivational affix ‑el(e)n acquired an iterative meaning has remained unclear. The same holds true for iterative semantics of verbs ending in ‑er(e)n, mainly onomatopoetic words denoting sounds, particularly sounds of animals (ger schnattern, zwitschern, dut snateren, kwetteren, eng chatter, twitter). Sharing iterative semantics, both suffixes are partly competing (ger schlittern, Alemannic schlitteln ‘to slither’; dut gaggelen, gakkeren ‘to cackle’).

Whereas recent schema-based approaches (Weidhaas & Schmid 2015, Audring et al. 2017) have focused on modern languages, the present account aims at shedding new light on the rise of iterative semantics from a diachronic perspective. It is argued that, primarily, the suffixes ‑el(e)n, ‑er(e)n go back to verbs derived from nomina instrumenti ending in –el, –er (ger Meißel, Stampfer, eng chisel, stamper, dut beitel, stamper). After reanalysis (‑el+en ® –elen; –er+en ® -eren) the word formation patterns became productive and gave rise to verb doublets (mlg stōtenstotteren) and onomatopoetic words (mlg snapperen ‘chatter’), see (1)-(2):

  1. mhg meiʒel ­ ‘chisel’ – meiʒel-en ->   meiʒ-elen ‘to chisel’
  2. mlg slenker ‘sling’ – slenker-en  –>   slenk-eren ‘to dangle’

   mhg snitzen ‘to carve’ – snitz-elen ‘to cut into pieces’

  mlg stōten ‘to exhale’ – stot(t)eren ‘to stammer’

Building on data drawn from historical dictionaries, it is argued that the semantics of the word formation patterns result from “affix telescoping”, i.e. formal and functional fusion of two derivational affixes (cf. Haspelmath 1995). In the cases in point, the semantics of ‑el(e)n/‑er(e)n-derivatives go back to -el/-er of nomina instrumenti with inherent iterative meaning. Other than l-diminutives, nomina instrumenti ending in ‑el, ‑er represent a common feature of the Germanic languages. Other sources, in particular diminutives and adjectival/comparative –er (dut beteren, ger bessern eng to better), are also discussed on the basis of historical data. The paper considers all three languages, however, the main focus is put on German and Dutch.

 

________________________________

References

 

Audring Jenny, Booij, Geert & Jackendoff, Ray (2017): Menscheln, kibbelen, sparkle. Verbal diminutives between grammar and lexicon. In: Linguistics in the Netherlands 34, 1–15.

De Jager, Arie (1875): Woordenboek der frequentatieven in het Nederlandsch. Gouda.

Eichinger, Ludwig M. (2014): Bairische Wortbildung. Verben auf -eln. In: Krämer-Neubert, Sabine & Wolf, Norbert Richard (eds.): Bayerische Dialektologie: Akten der Internationalen Dialektologischen Konferenz 26.–28. Februar 2002. Heidelberg, 63–73.

Haspelmath, Martin (1995): The growth of affixes in morphological reanalysis. In: Booi, Geert & van Marle, Jaap (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1994. Dordrecht, 1–29

Jurafsky, Daniel (1996): Universal Tendencies in the Semantics of the Diminutive. In: Language 72/3, 533–578.

Kuhn, Hans (1961): Verbale 1– und r-Bildungen im Schweizerdeutschen. Frauenfeld.

Weidhaas, Thomas/Schmid, Hans-Jörg (2015). Diminutive verbs in German: semantic analysis and theoretical implications. Morphology 25/2. 183–228.